Materials Map

Discover the materials research landscape. Find experts, partners, networks.

  • About
  • Privacy Policy
  • Legal Notice
  • Contact

The Materials Map is an open tool for improving networking and interdisciplinary exchange within materials research. It enables cross-database search for cooperation and network partners and discovering of the research landscape.

The dashboard provides detailed information about the selected scientist, e.g. publications. The dashboard can be filtered and shows the relationship to co-authors in different diagrams. In addition, a link is provided to find contact information.

×

Materials Map under construction

The Materials Map is still under development. In its current state, it is only based on one single data source and, thus, incomplete and contains duplicates. We are working on incorporating new open data sources like ORCID to improve the quality and the timeliness of our data. We will update Materials Map as soon as possible and kindly ask for your patience.

To Graph

1.080 Topics available

To Map

977 Locations available

693.932 PEOPLE
693.932 People People

693.932 People

Show results for 693.932 people that are selected by your search filters.

←

Page 1 of 27758

→
←

Page 1 of 0

→
PeopleLocationsStatistics
Naji, M.
  • 2
  • 13
  • 3
  • 2025
Motta, Antonella
  • 8
  • 52
  • 159
  • 2025
Aletan, Dirar
  • 1
  • 1
  • 0
  • 2025
Mohamed, Tarek
  • 1
  • 7
  • 2
  • 2025
Ertürk, Emre
  • 2
  • 3
  • 0
  • 2025
Taccardi, Nicola
  • 9
  • 81
  • 75
  • 2025
Kononenko, Denys
  • 1
  • 8
  • 2
  • 2025
Petrov, R. H.Madrid
  • 46
  • 125
  • 1k
  • 2025
Alshaaer, MazenBrussels
  • 17
  • 31
  • 172
  • 2025
Bih, L.
  • 15
  • 44
  • 145
  • 2025
Casati, R.
  • 31
  • 86
  • 661
  • 2025
Muller, Hermance
  • 1
  • 11
  • 0
  • 2025
Kočí, JanPrague
  • 28
  • 34
  • 209
  • 2025
Šuljagić, Marija
  • 10
  • 33
  • 43
  • 2025
Kalteremidou, Kalliopi-ArtemiBrussels
  • 14
  • 22
  • 158
  • 2025
Azam, Siraj
  • 1
  • 3
  • 2
  • 2025
Ospanova, Alyiya
  • 1
  • 6
  • 0
  • 2025
Blanpain, Bart
  • 568
  • 653
  • 13k
  • 2025
Ali, M. A.
  • 7
  • 75
  • 187
  • 2025
Popa, V.
  • 5
  • 12
  • 45
  • 2025
Rančić, M.
  • 2
  • 13
  • 0
  • 2025
Ollier, Nadège
  • 28
  • 75
  • 239
  • 2025
Azevedo, Nuno Monteiro
  • 4
  • 8
  • 25
  • 2025
Landes, Michael
  • 1
  • 9
  • 2
  • 2025
Rignanese, Gian-Marco
  • 15
  • 98
  • 805
  • 2025

Vangkilde, Signe

  • Google
  • 3
  • 8
  • 32

University of Copenhagen

in Cooperation with on an Cooperation-Score of 37%

Topics

Publications (3/3 displayed)

  • 2016Visual Attention in Posterior Stroke and Relations to Alexia11citations
  • 2015Cue-it? We say: Block-it!3citations
  • 2013Don't words come easy? A psychophysical exploration of word superiority18citations

Places of action

Chart of shared publication
Delfi, Tzvetelina
1 / 1 shared
Petersen, Anders
3 / 5 shared
Starrfelt, Randi
2 / 5 shared
Fabricius, Charlotte
1 / 1 shared
Iversen, Helle Klingenberg
1 / 1 shared
Wiegand, Iris
1 / 1 shared
Bundesen, Claus
1 / 1 shared
Cooreman, Bart
1 / 1 shared
Chart of publication period
2016
2015
2013

Co-Authors (by relevance)

  • Delfi, Tzvetelina
  • Petersen, Anders
  • Starrfelt, Randi
  • Fabricius, Charlotte
  • Iversen, Helle Klingenberg
  • Wiegand, Iris
  • Bundesen, Claus
  • Cooreman, Bart
OrganizationsLocationPeople

document

Cue-it? We say: Block-it!

  • Petersen, Anders
  • Vangkilde, Signe
  • Wiegand, Iris
  • Bundesen, Claus
  • Cooreman, Bart
Abstract

A bilateral change detection paradigm is often used to measure lateralized ERP-components, such as the Contralateral Delay Activity (CDA), believed to be associated with visual short-term memory (e.g. Vogel and Machizawa, 2004; Alvarez and Cavanagh, 2004; McCollough et al., 2007 ). Recently, Wiegand et al. (2014) developed a similar whole report paradigm in which participants reported the identity of four letters, presented in a pre-cued hemifield, showing a correlation between CDA amplitude and visual short-term memory capacity when modeled using Bundesen' s Theory of Visual Attention (TVA) (Bundesen, 1990) - in line with earlier findings suggesting that individuals with larger visual working memory capacity have larger CDA amplitudes than lower-capacity individuals (Vogel and Machizawa, 2004). In our EEG-study, we modeled healthy participants' visual attention performance in two versions of a five-letter whole report: a pre-cued version, similar to the paradigm used in Wiegand et al. (2014) in which a hundred percent valid symbolic cue preceded the letter display, and a blocked version, in which all letters in a given block were shown on the same side of the screen. The behavioral data were modelled by TVA, providing an estimate of perceptual threshold, processing speed, and visual short-term memory capacity for each participant. Our results show that the blocked design compared with the intermixed pre-cued design provided an equally good estimate of participants' TVA-parameters, and an equally prominent CDA. A clear advantage of the blocked design, however, is that participants seems to make less horizontal eye-movements compared with the intermixed design, and most important, the activity preceding our stimulus display is reduced in absence of a pre-cue, resulting in a more reliable baseline activity. A blocked design might therefore be considered a valid (and perhaps even slightly superior) alternative for cuing.

Topics
  • impedance spectroscopy
  • theory
  • laser emission spectroscopy